Pubdate: Tue, 03 Sep 2002 Source: Independent (UK) Copyright: 2002 Independent Newspapers (UK) Ltd. Contact: http://www.independent.co.uk/ Details: http://www.mapinc.org/media/209 Author: Charles Arthur, Technology Editor, Additional research by Sara Nelson Bookmark: http://www.mapinc.org/mdma.htm (Ecstasy) News Analysis: IS ECSTASY HARMFUL? SCIENTISTS STRUGGLE TO PROVIDE AN ANSWER Despite high-profile cases such as the death of Leah Betts, one million people a week still take Britain's favourite dance drug Exactly what does ecstasy do to the brain? And are those effects good, bad, or indifferent? To the estimated one million people who take the drug each weekend, one could guess that the answers are: it gives you energy to dance; you feel "loved up"; it's good. But a growing body of scientific evidence suggests that it's bad in the long term and almost always bad in the short term. But many of the people who take "E" - which has enjoyed a dancefloor revival in the past year or so - do not heed those warnings. They may have even taken heart from the reports about an article published in The Psychologist, the official journal of the British Psychological Society. The article suggests that it's hard to say whether MDMA - standing for 3,4-methylanedioxymethamphetamine, the amphetamine constituent of ecstasy - is really harmful, and that the people who show most of the milder effects ascribed to the drug - panic attacks, depression or anxiety - might simply be showing the symptoms of problems that they would have anyway. That's because they are young adults, reaching the age when any psychological illness could be expected to show through stress. The researchers insisted yesterday that they were not saying ecstasy was safe or without harmful effects. "The newspaper reports about our article were inaccurate," insisted a co-author, Dr Harry Sumnall, a postdoctoral researcher at the University of Liverpool. "We did not say that ecstasy is harmless. We explicitly say that it is dangerous." He pointed out that the article was not based on fresh research; it was a review of other research, and quotes scientific papers published since 1990. And the article itself was hardly freshly minted; it had been written 18 months ago, Dr Sumnall said. That meant it could not include some more recent research - such as that which appeared earlier this year, investigating a study published in 1998. That 1998 study included pictures of brain scans which apparently demonstrated that ecstasy destroys nerve cells involved in the production and transport of serotonin, a vital brain chemical involved in a wide range of functions including memory, sleep, sex, appetite, and primarily, mood. On PET brain scans, which make it possible to view a brain at work, those for non-users showed large "bright" regions, but those of ecstasy users showed fewer such regions - suggesting nerve damage. The pictures were adopted in anti-drugs advertising, and the research findings used to underpin stiffer penalties for ecstasy use. But in April the doubts emerged: it turned out that brains vary enormously when studied in that way. Stephen Kish, a neuropathologist at the Centre for Addiction and Health in Toronto, told New Scientist magazine: "There are no holes in the brains of ecstasy users. And if anyone wants a straightforward answer to whether ecstasy causes any brain damage, it's impossible to get one from these papers. "So, should we reconsider our attitude to ecstasy? In May, the Home Secretary David Blunkett was blunt. "Reclassification of ecstasy is not on the Government's agenda. Ecstasy can, and does, kill unpredictably and there is no such thing as a safe dose." Scientists are reluctant to be as didactic. Science, unlike politics, allows for uncertainty, and constantly re-evaluates its ideas. But at the same time, the scientific community that is studying ecstasy is "converging" on a set of theories about the drug which are consistent and which keep coming through study after study, said John Henry, professor of accident and emergency medicine at St Mary's Medical School, London, who is one of Britain's foremost experts on the subject. It is not good. It is not even neutral. Ecstasy does harm you, in subtle ways. "The short-term effects are very clear, because they're more measurable than long-term ones," he said. "The Government's pitch is about the deaths due to ecstasy, which are actually very small in number - though very serious, because these are all young people who were doing something recreational that's not regarded as dangerous in itself." The Psychologist article, he believes, is being "picky" from among the vast body of research that has been conducted into ecstasy over the past 20 years. People have wanted to believe it is harmless, and that the Government was wrong to ban it in 1977. And for some time in the 1980s when usage took off, it seemed like ecstasy could do no wrong, or at least little harm. Dr Sumnall points out that the drug does not cause dependency in the way it is usually defined. "You don't see [physical] dependency in ecstasy users," he said. "Although people might start to rely on it to have a good time." At that stage it could pass over to a different sort of reliance. But no scientist would ever claim that it could be harder to give up ecstasy than, for example, heroin, cocaine or crack cocaine, which generate a physical craving. While that may be true, the suggestion that the case against ecstasy is unproven raised the ire of other scientists in The Psychologist. "The article tends to ignore the overwhelming evidence that regular ecstasy users suffer from impulsive behaviour and deficits in verbal memory performance," said Michael Morgan, senior lecturer in experimental psychology at the University of Sussex. And Rodney Croft, a research fellow at the Centre for Neuropsychopharmacology at Swinburne University of Technology, Australia, said: "There is strong converging evidence that ecstasy does cause some [neuron] impairment." Professor Henry said that to attempt to ameliorate the potential effects of ecstasy based on a selective reading of research was wrong. "To suggest that its effects are all in the mind, that if you read about things like panic attacks and depression and memory loss then you're more likely to get them - well, there's plenty of evidence that adverse effects do occur." He pointed particularly to a study in Canada which followed a large number of users and non-users, and subjected them to regular memory tests. That found that the regular ecstasy users suffered from memory loss. "Even if they weren't aware of it, it was there," he said. "Your ability to function as a normal member of society is being nibbled away. I'm not saying it's some sort of premature Alzheimer's; but their ability to organise themselves and do things is being impaired, long term. And if that's happening nationwide, over a large proportion of the population - that's very bad news." - --- MAP posted-by: Richard Lake