Pubdate: Sat, 1 Mar 2008
Source: Vancouver Sun (CN BC)
Copyright: 2008 The Vancouver Sun
Contact:  http://www.canada.com/vancouver/vancouversun/
Details: http://www.mapinc.org/media/477
Author: Peter McKnight, Vancouver Sun
Cited: British Columbia Centre for Excellence in HIV/AIDS 
http://www.cfenet.ubc.ca
Bookmark: http://www.mapinc.org/topic/Insite (Insite)
Bookmark: http://www.mapinc.org/find?142 (Supervised Injection Sites)

WHEN SCIENCE RUNS INTO AN IDEOLOGICAL WALL

The Canadian Government Has Been Taken to Task for Its Lack of 
Support For, or Knowledge Of, Scientific Research

While it's usually a badge of distinction to have your work cited in 
a top-flight academic journal, the federal government wasn't exactly 
in a celebratory mood after two recent journal editorials discussed 
the feds' attitude toward science.

That's because the journals had little good to say about the 
government's lack of support for, or knowledge of, scientific 
research. As far as lack of support is concerned, Nature magazine 
cited the government's recent decision to eliminate its science 
adviser position, its muzzling of Environment Canada scientists, and 
its putative failure to adequately fund research as evidence of "the 
government's manifest disregard for science."

And as for a lack of knowledge, scientists at the B.C. Centre for 
Excellence in HIV/AIDS took Health Minister Tony Clement to task in 
The Lancet Infectious Diseases for Clement's apparent inability to 
distinguish between peer-reviewed medical literature and an opinion 
piece appearing on the website of a lobby group.

The Lancet Infectious Diseases article followed an earlier editorial 
published last year in the online journal Open Medicine. That 
editorial, written by University of Toronto medical professor Stephen 
Hwang and endorsed by more than 130 scientists, argued that the 
government's approach to Vancouver's supervised injection site 
reveals "that scientific evidence is about to be trumped by ideology."

These are damning charges. And there is no question that the 
government has been less than supportive of any scientific evidence 
that conflicts with its ideology. This is a serious problem, since 
preferring to see the world as you think it ought to be (ideology) 
instead of the way it is (the scientific evidence) can be fatal, not 
just for governments, but for everyone.

Yet there is substantial evidence that ideology influences our 
assessment of scientific evidence, particularly when one's views are 
ideologically entrenched.

In one experiment, social psychologist Charles Lord divided students 
into two groups -- one made up those who were the most ardent 
supporters of capital punishment, and the other of the most ardent 
opponents of the death penalty.

Lord then gave half of the students in each group a set of studies 
showing that the death penalty acted as a deterrent, and the other 
half in each group received studies showing that capital punishment 
had no deterrent effect.

Now, were the students acting rationally, we would expect those who 
received evidence contrary to their views to soften their positions 
somewhat. But the opposite happened -- both the supporters and 
opponents of capital punishment strengthened their views upon 
receiving contrary evidence.

In effect, the students explained away the contrary evidence -- and 
justified their original positions -- by criticizing the methods of 
those studies that failed to support their ideologies.

More recently, Donald Braman and Dan Kahan of Yale University, in a 
paper titled More Statistics, Less Persuasion: A Cultural Theory of 
Gun Risk Perceptions, found that people's positions on gun control 
are determined by their cultural worldviews. Much as in the death 
penalty study, the researchers concluded that "individuals can be 
expected to credit or dismiss empirical evidence on 'gun control 
risks' depending on whether it coheres or conflicts with their 
cultural values."

These are troubling findings because they suggest people behave in a 
manner exactly the opposite of that prescribed by science, which 
dictates that we test and modify our theories on the basis of the 
evidence, rather than interpreting the evidence in light of our theories.

But given that this is how many people -- and many governments -- 
behave, and given the importance of allowing scientific evidence to 
inform government policy, it's essential that we find ways of 
developing a rapprochement between researchers and policy-makers.

Fortunately, there is a wealth of literature on what is called 
"research transfer" or "knowledge utilization." Much of this 
literature has been written by Canadians, including many in the 
employ of the federal government.

In one important paper titled Connecting Research and Policy, 
Canadian Health Services Research Foundation CEO Jonathan Lomas notes 
that many factors influence government decision-making, including 
interests (how one would like the world to work), ideologies (how one 
thinks the world ought to work) and beliefs (how one thinks the world 
actually does work.)

Of these three, Lomas argues that beliefs are the only factor likely 
to be changed as a result of information, and even then, information 
comprises much more than just scientific research, as it also 
includes "anecdotes, experience and even propaganda." Further, 
beliefs typically take a long time -- often years -- to change, and 
then only after "repeated exposure from competing sources of information."

Given this reality, how can we make governments more responsive to 
scientific research? Perhaps most importantly, Lomas notes both 
researchers and policy-makers must have a better understanding of 
each others' domains.

In particular, both scientists and decision-makers tend to view the 
others' field as a product rather than a process. Government 
policy-makers, for example, typically see science as a "retail store" 
that provides them with just the product they need when they want it.

A good example of this view came from Tony Clement when he 
begrudgingly extended the supervised injection site's lease on life, 
saying that he needed more "facts" about the site's effect on 
lowering drug use and fighting addiction.

While science can provide such information, Clement's words reveal 
that he sees science as a retail store rather than as an activity, a process.

The problem with this approach is that it virtually guarantees that 
researchers and policy-makers will come into contact with each other 
only at the moment a decision is made, and researchers will present 
their findings only after the policy agenda "has been framed within a 
particular context . . . and often after the limits have been set 
around feasible options."

And as the government's approach to the supervised injection site -- 
and the study of students and the death penalty -- make clear, it's 
highly unlikely scientific research will change the beliefs of 
policy-makers at such a late stage in the process, particularly when 
the government holds ideologically entrenched views.

If, on the other hand, policy-makers view science as a process, and 
maintain regular contact with scientists, they can influence the 
"conceptualization and conduct of a study" and are also more likely 
to allow the study's results to inform policy.

Similarly, if scientists view policy-making as a process, and 
maintain regular contact with policy-makers, they stand a much better 
chance of influencing the policy agenda and framing the issues, which 
again increases the chances that their results will inform policy.

For these reasons, the Canadian government has placed considerable 
emphasis on research transfer, and has developed many linkages 
between researchers and policy-makers. This close relationship might 
explain why many bureaucrats within the government have been 
influenced by the research on the supervised injection site.

The problem, of course, is that government policy is ultimately set 
by the cabinet ministers and the prime minister, who continue to view 
science as a retail store. And until that changes, the government 
will continue to make the pages of academic journals, for all the 
wrong reasons. 
- ---
MAP posted-by: Richard Lake